Analysis of Stingless Bee Aggression

By Dylan Voeller and James Nieh

(T. spinipes forager attacking M. rufiventris forager at a food source)

 

I.          Principles introduced in this exercise

A.        Aggression: how they attack

B.        Defender's perspective: what to attack

C.        Results of aggression

 

II.        Introduction

The social regulation of aggression plays an important role in competitive interactions among animals ( Moynihan 1998 ); the structure of arthropod communities ( Reitz and Trumble 2002 ); invasion ecology ( Holway et al. 2002 ); and the evolution of animal behavior ( Maynard-Smith and Harper 1988 ). However, questions remain concerning the detailed effects of group size on individual aggression during aggressive encounters between groups ( Archer 1988 ), even in primates ( Bernstein and Ehardt 1985; Camperio 1986; Goodall 1990; Stanford et al. 1994; Gros-Louis et al. 2003 ).

 

In social insects, group aggression plays a vital role in ants, social bees, and social wasps during nest defense and, in some species, during foraging ( Wilson 1971; Michener 1974; Breed et al. 1990; Hölldobler and Wilson 1990 ). For example, Wilson ( 1971 ) reported that individual Ancanthomyops claviger ants increased their sensitivity to alarm odors when in larger groups. Interspecific competition over limited food sources has led to the evolution of aggressive group recruitment systems in stingless bees (Hymenoptera, Apidae, Meliponini), one of the most important native Neotropical pollinators ( Johnson and Hubbell 1974; Johnson and Hubbell 1987; Roubik 1989; Nagamitsu and Inoue 1997; Slaa et al. 1997; Kevan and Imperatriz-Fonseca 2002; Slaa 2003; Nieh et al. 2004 ). Such aggressive competition may be costly and critical to colony survival, especially during periods of relative food dearth ( Roubik 1982; Roubik 1989 ). Johnson & Hubbell ( 1974 ) recorded 63% mortality (1812 dead bees) after a two-day battle between three colonies of T. corvina ( Lepeletier 1835 ) over sucrose solution baits. 

 

However, relatively little is known about the detailed patterns of aggressive behavior ( Johnson 1974 ) that constitute meliponine dominance styles, or the strategies and behaviors that individuals or groups typically employ to attain dominance ( Cooper and Bernstein 2002 ).  This exercise focuses on defining behaviors involved in aggressive encounters between bee species in order to better understand these interactions.

 

III.             Materials & Methods

We are going to look at several movies which show bees battling over a food source: a sugar solution feeder (pink platform) or a flower.  The T. spinipes foragers are the smaller, more numerous black bees and the victim is a species of Melipona, in these videos the larger furry bee.  Unlike honeybees, stingless bees do not have stingers so they fight with their jaws (mandibles).

 

 

T. spinipes vs. honeybee
[Quicktime Movie]

T. spinipes vs. M. subnitida 3
[Quicktime Movie]

A.     Click on the link titled “T. hyalinata vs. M. rufiventris” to open the Quicktime movie.  Once the file has opened and begins to play, count the number of bees attacking the victim.   If the bees are moving too fast, first watch the video in real time, and then watch it frame-by-frame.  

 

 

1. Click on the pause button

 

 

2. Click on the forward or back button

The Aggressor

B.     *Choose one of the black bees (T. spinipes) and determine her attack intensity throughout the video.  Define the attack intensity using the following table.

Level I: low intensity threats (facing opponent and wide wingspread) without direct contact

Level II: brief body contact (for example, a quick bite)

Level III: extended biting and pulling

Level IV: wrestling (individuals are locked together by leg gripping and/or mandible contact)

Escape: attempting to walk or fly away while being restricted from moving away by attackers

*Definitions based on Johnson & Hubbell (1974), and Nieh et al. (2005).

 

Here is an example of a sample table you could draw up.  Each student can fill in a different column of the table.

 

Sample Table 1: Aggression classifications from T. hyalinata vs. M. rufiventris

Subject Bee

Attacker 1

Attacker 2

Attacker 3

Attackee 1

Level of Aggression

Level III

Level IV

Level II

Escape

Target Region

abdomen & thorax

head

abdomen & thorax

none

 

 

C.     Does the attack intensity of your bee change?  Does it increase or decrease?  If it does change, then make a new table showing that change at two different points in time.  For example, create a “beginning” table and an “end” table.

 

Sample Table 2: Aggression classifications from T. hyalinata vs. M. rufiventris

BEGINNING

Subject Bee

Attacker 1

Attacker 2

Attacker 3

Attackee 1

Level of Aggression

Level III

Level IV

Level II

Escape

Target Region

abdomen & thorax

head

abdomen & thorax

None

 

Sample Table 3: Aggression classifications from T. hyalinata vs. M. rufiventris

END

Subject Bee

Attacker 1

Attacker 2

Attacker 3

Attackee 1

Level of Aggression

Level II

Level III

Level II

Escape

Target Region

head

head

abdomen & thorax

none

 

The Victim

D.    Define the region of the body being targeted by each attacker as either “head” or “thorax and abdomen”.

E.     Repeat steps A though D for the remaining Quicktime movies. 

 

The Attack Strategy

Stingless bees, like honeybees, have certain attack preferences.  Stingless bees like to attack hairy objects and dark objects.  If you think about this, you will realize that bees are drawn towards attacking sensitive areas around the head of a predator.  When they attack each other stingless bees can try to cut off wings, legs, or even the head.  In fact, you can sometimes see bees flying around with their legs “decorated” with the severed heads of unsuccessful attackers.  This happens during group attacks when members of the colony defend the bee being attacked.

 

F.      Can you describe what kind of attack strategy you saw in each of the three videos?

G.    How did the victim respond?  Since there was only a single victim and several attackers, what would be the best strategy for the victim?

H.    If the victim is trying to escape and the attackers want to prevent this, are there any strategies that the attackers should use?

 

IV.              Sample Results

In the T. hyalinata vs. M. rufiventris video we observed:

*Note the positions of the play bar which tell you where the aggression occurred in the video

 

Level I aggression

(A T. hyalinata forager spreads its wings while moving towards the M. rufiventris victim)

 

Level II aggression

(A T. hyalinata forager executes a quick bite as the M. rufiventris forager tries to run away)

 

Level III aggression

(A T. hyalinata forager remains on M. rufiventris while biting for several seconds)

 

Level IV aggression

(A T. hyalinata forager and M. rufiventris victim wrestle with locked mandibles for an extended period of time)

 

Escape

(M. rufiventris attempts to run away while several T. hyalinata foragers follow behind)

 

 

V.        Sample Discussion Questions

 

A.     How did the bees attack?

 

B.     Was there anything in particular that attackers focused on?

 

C.     Why might some body parts be attacked over others?

 

D.    What were the results of the aggression?  Did the attacker run away?

 

E.     What does it mean to have a higher level of aggression?

 

F.      What kinds of animals do you think this kind of attack would be effective on? 

 

G.    Have you ever seen a bee attack before or been attacked? 

 

H.    How do you think the bees evolved their particular strategy of attacks?

 

VI.       Sample Conclusions

A group of T. spinipes bees (attackers) dominated a single larger Melipona sp. bee (attackee) in each movie.  It appears that the use of aggression by the T. spinipes bees in larger numbers may be an effective strategy for warding off the larger Melipona sp. bees.  It is possible that this aggression strategy could have evolved because attacking in groups allowed the smaller bees to win more “battles” and therefore gain access to more food resources.  It is also possible that attacking in groups decreases the chances of injury for the smaller bees, as escalated aggression levels are more likely to lead to serious injury.  The attackers did not seem to focus on any one part of the body more often than the other, but there was almost always more than one bee attacking at any one time.

 

References

Archer, J., 1988. The behavioural biology of aggression. New York, Cambridge University Press,

257.

Bernstein, I.S., 1981. Dominance: the baby and the bathwater. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:

419-457.

Breed, M.D., Robinson, G.E., and Page Jr., R.E., 1990. Division of labor during honey bee

colony defense. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 27: 395-401.

Camperio, C.A., 1986. Intertroop agonistic behavior of a feral rhesus macaque troop ranging

in town and forest areas in India. Aggressive Behavior 12: 433-439.

Cooper, M.A., Bernstein, I.S., 2002. Counter aggression and reconciliation in Assamese

macaques (Macaca assamensis). American Journal of Primatology 56: 215-230.

Goodall, J., 1990. Through a window: thirty years with the chimpanzees of Gombe. London,

Weidenfield & Nicholson, 288.

Gros-Louis, J., Perry, S., and Manson, J.H., 2003. Violent coalitionary attacks and

intraspecific killing in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Primates

44: 341-346.

Hölldobler, B., and Wilson, E.O., 1990. The ants. Cambridge, Massachusetts, Belknap Press

of Harvard University Press, 732.

Holway, D.A., Lach, L., Suarez, A.V., Tsutsui, N.D., and Case, T.J., 2002. The causes and

consequences of ant invasions. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 33: 181-233.

Johnson, L.K., 1974. The role of agonistic behavior in the foraging strategies of Trigona bees.

Berkeley, California, USA, PhD thesis, University of California Berkeley.

Johnson, L.K. and Hubbell, S.P., 1974. Aggression and competition among stingless bees:

field studies. Ecology 55: 120-127.

Johnson, L.K., and Hubbell, S.P., 1987. Defense of a food supply by eusocial colonies.

American Zoologist 27: 347-358.

Kevan, P.G., and Imperatriz-Fonseca, V.L., 2002. Pollinating bees: the conservation link between

agriculture and nature. Proceedings of the workshop on the conservation and

sustainable use of pollinators in agriculture, with emphasis on  bees, São Paulo,

Brazil, Ministry of Environment, Bárbara bela, Editora Gráfica, 313.

Lepeletier, S.-F.D., 1835. Histoire naturelle d’insectes-hyménoptéres. Paris, Roret, 1-547.

Maynard-Smith, J. and Harper, D.G.C., 1988. The evolution of aggression: can selection

generate variability? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B:

Biological Sciences 319: 557-570.

Michener, C.D., 1974. The social behavior of the bees. Cambridge, Mass, Harvard University

Press.

Moynihan, M., 1998. The social regulation of competition and aggression in animals. Washington,

Smithsonian Institution Press, 158.

Nagamitsu, T., and Inoue, T., 1997. Aggressive foraging of social bees as a mechanism of

floral resource partitioning in an Asian tropical rainforest. Oecologia 110: 432-439.

Nieh, J.C., Barreto, L.S., Contrera, F.A.L., and Imperatriz-Fonseca, V.L., 2004. Olfactory

eavesdropping by a competitively foraging stingless bee, Trigona spinipes. Proceedings of

the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 271: 1633-1640.

Nieh, J.C., Kruizinga, K., Barreto, L.S., Contrera, F.A.L., and Imperatriz-Fonseca, V.L.,

2005. Effect of group size on the aggression strategy of an extirpating stingless bee,

Trigona spinipes. Insectes Sociaux, in press.

Reitz, S.R. and Trumble, J.T., 2002. Competative displacement among insects and arachnids.

Annual Review of Entomology 47, 435-465.

Roubik, D.W., 1982. Seasonality in colony food storage, brood production, and adult

survivorship: studies of Melipona in tropical forest (Hyminoptera: Apidae). Journal of the

Kansas Entomological Society 55: 789-800.

Roubik, D.W., 1989. Ecology and natural history of tropical bees.  New York, Cambridge

University Press, 514.

Slaa, E.J., 2003. Foraging ecology of stingless bees: from individual behaviour to community

ecology. PhD thesis, Department of Behavioural Biology. Utrecht, Utrecht University: 181.

Slaa, E.J., van Nieuwstadt, M.G.L., Pisa, L.W. and Sommeijer, M.J., 1997. Foraging strategies

of stingless bees (Apidae Meliponinae): the relation between precision recruitment,

competition and communication. Acta Horticulturae 437: 193-197.

Stanford, C.B., Wallis, J.B., Matama, H., and Goodall, J., 1994. Patterns of predation by

chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe National Park. American Journal of

Physical Anthropology 94: 213-228.

Wilson, E.O., 1971. The insect societies. Cambridge, Mass, Belknap Press of Harvard University

Press, 548.